Archives

  • 2018-07
  • 2018-10
  • 2018-11
  • 2019-04
  • 2019-05
  • 2019-06
  • 2019-07
  • 2019-08
  • 2019-09
  • 2019-10
  • 2019-11
  • 2019-12
  • 2020-01
  • 2020-02
  • 2020-03
  • 2020-04
  • 2020-05
  • 2020-06
  • 2020-07
  • 2020-08
  • 2020-09
  • 2020-10
  • 2020-11
  • 2020-12
  • 2021-01
  • 2021-02
  • 2021-03
  • 2021-04
  • 2021-05
  • 2021-06
  • 2021-07
  • 2021-08
  • 2021-09
  • 2021-10
  • 2021-11
  • 2021-12
  • 2022-01
  • 2022-02
  • 2022-03
  • 2022-04
  • 2022-05
  • 2022-06
  • 2022-07
  • 2022-08
  • 2022-09
  • 2022-10
  • 2022-11
  • 2022-12
  • 2023-01
  • 2023-02
  • 2023-03
  • 2023-04
  • 2023-05
  • 2023-06
  • 2023-07
  • 2023-08
  • 2023-09
  • 2023-10
  • 2023-11
  • 2023-12
  • 2024-01
  • 2024-02
  • 2024-03
  • 2024-04
  • br Methods br Results br Discussion br Introduction The use

    2018-11-07


    Methods
    Results
    Discussion
    Introduction The use of institutional care for abandoned or orphaned infants and children has remained a common practice throughout the world (Browne et al., 2006; UNICEF, 2015); however, institutional environments are harmful for healthy physical and psychological development especially for young children. Many institutions have high child-to-caregiver ratios, have highly regimented schedules with extended periods where children are left alone, and very little engagement with the caregivers (Castle et al., 1999; Nelson, 2007; Smyke et al., 2007). The consequences of these rearing conditions often include physical growth restriction, a wide range of behavioral problems, and deficits in cognitive function compared to children raised in families (Maclean, 2003; Nelson, 2007; for review, see Nelson et al., 2014). Extreme deprivation dramatically alters neural architecture and functioning. A number of neuroimaging studies have examined the impact of those early experiences in post-institutionalized adoptees. For example, structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have shown decreased grey and white matter volumes (Mehta et al., 2009; Sheridan et al., 2012), increased amygdala volume (Mehta et al., 2009; Tottenham et al., 2010), decreased cerebellar volumes (Bauer et al., 2009), and disrupted connectivity between the frontal and temporal lobes (Eluvathingal et al., 2006) in previously institutionalized children compared to community controls. Together these studies demonstrate that typical neural development is altered as a result of early deprivation. However, fewer studies have examined the impact of intervention and tracked changes in CAL-101 Supplier development as a result of removal from institutions. The BEIP is the first study to experimentally examine the physical, psychological, and neural sequelae of institutional rearing and the developmental trajectories of children removed and placed into a novel foster care intervention (Smyke et al., 2009; Zeanah et al., 2003). A group of infants living in institutions as well as an age-matched community control sample (never institutionalized group; NIG) all living in Bucharest, Romania were recruited into the study. After an initial screening to exclude for genetic disorders or other medical conditions the infants completed a baseline assessment, including resting EEG. The infants were then randomly assigned to either remain in the institution (care as usual; CAUG) or placed with a foster family (FCG). Random assignment of these children provides an opportunity to examine the effects of the intervention and repeated assessments over the children\'s life have allowed examination of changes in development associated with enrichment of their early care environment. Greater detail regarding the study design and ethical issues are described elsewhere (Miller, 2009; Millum and Emanuel, 2007; Zeanah et al., 2012). Previous reports from this study found at the baseline assessment, infants living in institutions showed markedly decreased power in both alpha and beta activity and increased theta power compared to the never institutionalized community controls (Marshall, Fox and the BEIP core group, 2004). The pattern of higher theta and lower alpha power is one that is associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and other learning disorders (Barry et al., 2003, 2009; Chabot et al., CAL-101 Supplier 2005; McLaughlin et al., 2010), the former being highly prevalent among previously institutionalized children (Bos et al., 2011; Kreppner et al., 2001; Wiik et al., 2011; Zeanah et al., 2009). Following placement into foster care, EEG was again collected when the children were 42 months. The mean age of placement for the foster care group was 22 months. At the 42 month assessment there was a hint that earlier placement into foster care was beneficial, as infants placed at younger ages showed increasing alpha power relative to older placed infants (Marshall et al., 2008). The effect of timing of placement in foster care became clear by the time the children were 8-years-old; infants placed into foster care before 24 months were indistinguishable from children in the community while those placed after 24 months were identical to the CAUG (Vanderwert et al., 2010). Further, age of removal from institutions also significantly impacted the developmental trajectories of alpha and beta power between 42-months and 8-years (Stamoulis et al., 2015) suggesting ongoing plasticity in beta rhythm that was not detected in the follow-up at 8 years.